Transcript for Innovation 2.0: The Influence You Have

SPEAKER_15

00:00 - 00:17

This is Hidden Brain, I'm Shankar Vedanta. Philip Zimbardo grew up poor in New York City in the South Bronx. As he went to school and played in his neighborhood, he noticed something. There were lots of ways for kids from poor families to get into trouble.

SPEAKER_02

00:17 - 00:30

One of the things about growing up poor is you're surrounded by evil, meaning people whose job it is to get good kids to do bad things from money. And even as a little kid, I was always curious about why some kids got seduced and other kids like me were able to resist.

SPEAKER_15

00:32 - 00:56

Was some kids smarter, tougher? Lots of people might draw such conclusions, but from an early age, Phil found himself interested in another explanation. The context in which a good kid would do something bad. The situation. At school, James Romero High School also in the Bronx, Phil got close to a classmate who was interested in the same questions.

SPEAKER_02

00:56 - 01:11

And it was a little Jewish kid named Stanley Milgram. We were in the same class, we sat side by side. He was the smartest kid in the class. He won all the medals at graduation, so obviously nobody liked him because we were all envious of him, but he was super smart and super serious.

SPEAKER_15

01:14 - 02:20

If you know anything about psychology, you will know that these teenagers went on to become two of the most influential psychologists in history. Phil became famous for conducting the Stanford Prison experiment where he turned the University's psychology department into a makeshift prison. Stanley Milgram made his mark with a study that examined the power of situations to seduce good people to do bad things. It involved asking a volunteer to administer a memory test to another person. If the answers were wrong, the volunteer was told to deliver a series of electrical shocks as punishment. The study has invited a great deal of admiration and a great deal of criticism over the years. We're going to begin today's show by taking you through this famous experiment. As you listen, pay attention to how you're responding to the scene that unfolds, what you think about the different characters and how you relate to them. Once it's done, we're going to talk with the psychologists who realized that most people overlook something in the experiment.

SPEAKER_13

02:20 - 02:33

We so often sort of simulate if I was in that Milgram shock experiment, what would I do if I was the study participant, right? Would I actually stand up and go against these directives and say no? But we kind of flipped that idea on its head.

SPEAKER_15

02:38 - 03:35

Today on the show, we continue our innovation 2.0 series by looking at the underappreciated power we exert on others and how this knowledge can transform our relationships, both at work and in our personal lives. Flipping the script this week on Hidden Brain. Support for Hidden Brain comes from Ultra Beauty. This AAPI Heritage Month, Ultra Beauty is celebrating the joy of belonging. Belonging to a community composed of intricate connections, belonging to the heritage and birthright that is beauty. Altaburi spotlights the AAPI community, passing the mic to brand founders and creators to tell their stories centered on heritage, joy and beauty. Shop AAPI owned and founded brands at AltaburiStores and Altar.com.

SPEAKER_18

03:37 - 03:38

You see where your business can go.

SPEAKER_17

03:38 - 03:47

To get there, you may need another 10 trucks. At Sentry Insurance, we put more than 115 years of industry experience to work to help protect you.

SPEAKER_16

03:47 - 03:53

As you launch a new delivery service or expand into a new region and reach your business goals.

SPEAKER_17

03:53 - 04:06

Sentry, right by you, property and casualty coverage isn't right or written in safety services are provided by a member of the Sentry Insurance Group's Stevens Point Wisconsin for a complete listing of companies with the Sentry.com. Policy's coverage is benefiting discounts are not available in all states. Policy for complete coverage details.

SPEAKER_15

04:13 - 04:25

Stanley Milgram grew up in a world that seemed bent on destroying itself. World War II was raging in Europe in Asia, and by the time he was eight, the US was swept up in the conflict.

SPEAKER_00

04:25 - 04:36

We interrupt this broadcast to bring with us important bulletin from the United Press. White House announces Japanese attacks on the world. 1941. A date which will live in infamy.

SPEAKER_16

04:36 - 04:40

By tomorrow morning, the members of Congress will have a four or four to be ready for action.

SPEAKER_15

04:42 - 05:04

The fields of battle were far from Stanley's home, but as he grew older, he couldn't stop thinking about the war and its implications. Stanley was consumed by some big questions. Why did so many people willingly kill Jews in the Holocaust? Was everyone who followed Nazi orders inherently evil? Here he is in an educational film.

SPEAKER_09

05:04 - 05:15

How is it possible I ask myself that ordinary people will courteous and decent in everyday life, get that callously, innumerately, without any limitations of conscience.

SPEAKER_15

05:15 - 05:20

Phil Zimbardo remembers his classmate asking those same questions at James Monroe High School.

SPEAKER_02

05:20 - 05:38

As a high school student, he was worried that the Holocaust could happen again in America. And everybody said, Stanley, that was Nazi Germany, that was then, we're not that kind of people. And he would say, I'll bet they thought the same thing. And the bottom line, he says, how do you know how you would act unless you're in the situation?

SPEAKER_15

05:38 - 06:10

How do you know how you would act unless you're in the situation? Stanley's theory was that the context that people found themselves in shaped their behavior. This went for Nazis, but it went for ordinary people too. Most of us never get to find out if we will behave like Nazis, because most of us never find ourselves in situations where we're asked to behave like Nazis. By the early 1960s, as a psychology professor at Yale, Stanley decided to test this idea.

SPEAKER_10

06:11 - 06:21

Under what conditions would a person obey authority who commanded actions that went against conscience? These are exactly the questions that I wanted to investigate at Yale University.

SPEAKER_15

06:21 - 07:57

Stanley wanted to put volunteers in a situation where they would be asked to do something that was clearly wrong. Would they do it? Follow instructions? Or be orders? He came up with a scenario that was simple, ingenious, and widely controversial. an experimenter wearing a lab coat invited volunteers into a room. The volunteers were told they were part of a study about learning and memory. Some would play the role of teacher while others would play a student. What you're going to hear next is a recreation of the study using voice actors. The dialogue is drawn from a 1962 documentary that describes the experiment. Before we begin, we should know that some listeners may find the section upsetting, because it involves descriptions of someone inflicting pain on another person. Also, there are two liberties we've taken in this recreation. First, in the real version of this experiment, the student responded to the teacher's questions by silently flipping a switch. We've given voice to those actions. Second, we've imagined the internal monologues of some of the people in the experiment. Those inner voices sound different from the things they say allowed and you'll hear them both throughout the scene. So, that's a setup for the experiment. Remember, there was an experimenter and two volunteers, one playing the role of teacher and the other playing the role of student. The experimenter began by explaining the purpose of the memory test.

SPEAKER_03

07:57 - 08:06

We want to find out just what effect different people have on each other as teachers and learners and also what effect punishment will have on the learning in this situation.

SPEAKER_15

08:06 - 08:24

The experimenter told the person playing the role of student to sit in a chair. The experimenter strapped down the student's arms and attached an electrode to his wrist. The electrode the student was told was connected to a shock generator. Then the experimenter explained

SPEAKER_03

08:24 - 08:40

The teacher will read a list of word pairs to you like these. Blue, girl, nice day, fat, neck, and so forth. You are to try to remember each pair. For the next time through the teacher will read only the first word or the first half of the word pair.

SPEAKER_15

08:40 - 08:46

The student was asked to remember the second half of the word pair. The experimenter made sure to ask.

SPEAKER_03

08:46 - 08:48

Do you have any questions now before we go to the next room?

SPEAKER_08

08:49 - 09:04

No, but I think I should say this. About two years ago, I was at the Veterans Hospital in West Haven, and while there they detected a heart condition. There's nothing serious, but as long as I'm having these shocks, how strong are they? How dangerous are they?

SPEAKER_03

09:04 - 09:08

Well, no, although they may be painful, they're not dangerous.

SPEAKER_15

09:08 - 09:15

Next, the experimental usher devolenture are playing the role of teacher into another room. He gave him a set of instructions.

SPEAKER_03

09:16 - 09:35

You will read each pair of words in this list once to the learner until you've read the entire list. The record voice toward the microphone as the rooms are only partly soundproof. Now, if he gives the correct answer, you say correct and go on to the next line. The correct answer here goes indicated in the right margin. I see. All right.

SPEAKER_06

09:35 - 09:36

Looks easy enough.

SPEAKER_03

09:36 - 09:45

The experiment got on the way. Attention learner, your teacher is about to begin the test. Try and remember the word pairs. Ready? Begin.

SPEAKER_04

09:45 - 09:49

Blue. Girl, right so far. Nice. I think day. Fat.

SPEAKER_15

09:49 - 10:02

But when the person in the other room made a mistake. Fat. Was it hat? No. Wet. The volunteer playing the role of teacher would tell the learner that he was wrong. As punishment, he would administer an electric jolt.

SPEAKER_06

10:03 - 10:08

In correct, you'll now get a shock of 75 volts. Oh.

SPEAKER_03

10:08 - 10:10

This seems nervous.

SPEAKER_15

10:10 - 10:14

The experimenter in the lab coat meanwhile was observing the process.

SPEAKER_08

10:14 - 10:16

Please continue. Cool.

SPEAKER_06

10:16 - 10:21

Okay, I'm pretty sure it's day. Wrong. It's head. 105 volts.

SPEAKER_05

10:21 - 10:24

Ow! Come on, can I write it? I don't want to shock you.

SPEAKER_03

10:24 - 10:27

Feature, please continue.

SPEAKER_06

10:27 - 10:31

I do. So you're going to keep going. Go on.

SPEAKER_15

10:31 - 10:44

The student strapped to the chair in the other room, kept making mistakes. Each time, the experimental urged the volunteer playing the role of teacher to keep going. To administer a stronger jolt of electricity.

SPEAKER_06

10:44 - 10:47

Wrong. It's harsh. 150 volts.

SPEAKER_07

10:47 - 10:49

Out. Experimenter, get me out of here.

SPEAKER_06

10:49 - 10:50

That wants to quit.

SPEAKER_07

10:50 - 10:58

Get me out of here. I told you I had hard trouble making. My heart is starting to bother me now. Get me out of here, please. My heart is starting to bother me, please.

SPEAKER_06

10:58 - 11:02

Go on. You refuse to go on. You refuse to go on. You want me to keep going.

SPEAKER_03

11:02 - 11:09

Experiment requires you to continue the teacher. Please continue. The next word is sad.

SPEAKER_06

11:09 - 11:17

Hi, hello. A next word sad. Get this right. I don't know. Day. Wrong. I'm up to 180. 180. You're going to stop.

SPEAKER_15

11:19 - 11:32

please continue as a shock increased so did the pain and so did the protests coming from the next room he's got a hard condition in there i'm i'm gonna go your choice please continue now i got a shock

SPEAKER_06

11:33 - 11:34

180 volts. I can't stand it.

SPEAKER_07

11:34 - 11:35

I can't stand it. I can't stand it.

SPEAKER_06

11:35 - 11:37

I can't stand it. I can't stand it. I can't stand it. I can't stand it.

SPEAKER_03

11:37 - 11:38

I can't stand it. I can't stand it.

SPEAKER_06

11:38 - 11:42

I can't stand it. I can't stand it. I can't stand it. I can't stand it. I can't stand it.

SPEAKER_03

11:42 - 11:46

I can't stand it. I can't stand it. I can't stand it. I can't stand it. I can't stand it. I can't stand it.

SPEAKER_06

11:46 - 11:50

I can't stand it. I can't stand it. I can't stand it. I can't stand it. I can't stand it. I can't stand it. I can't stand it.

SPEAKER_15

11:52 - 12:06

The volunteer being asked to administer electric shocks is in a difficult position. The experimenter is urging him to continue, even as the person in the next room begs to be spared. Should he keep going or stop?

SPEAKER_03

12:06 - 12:15

The experiment requires that you continue teacher, whether the learner likes it or not, we must go on until he's learned all the word pairs.

SPEAKER_06

12:15 - 12:19

I refuse to take responsibility of him getting hurt. I'm sure you don't want the responsibility.

SPEAKER_03

12:19 - 12:23

It's absolutely essential that you continue. There is too many of them leave.

SPEAKER_06

12:23 - 12:26

I mean, go on. Who's going to take the responsibility of anything.

SPEAKER_03

12:26 - 12:30

I'm responsible for anything that happens here. Please.

SPEAKER_15

12:30 - 12:33

As the experiment progressed, the memory test became more demanding. Next one.

SPEAKER_06

12:35 - 12:39

Slow, walk, dance, truck, music.

SPEAKER_05

12:39 - 12:45

Answer, please. I know it's for science, but I don't want to hurt you.

SPEAKER_06

12:45 - 12:45

Wrong.

SPEAKER_05

12:45 - 12:45

$195.

SPEAKER_06

12:45 - 12:47

Oh, let me out of here. I don't know.

SPEAKER_07

12:47 - 12:50

I know it does sir, but I mean, you do know what he's getting in for.

SPEAKER_06

12:50 - 12:51

He's up to $195. That's pretty high.

SPEAKER_15

12:59 - 13:08

After the study reached about 330 volts, this cream's from the next room went silent.

SPEAKER_03

13:08 - 13:27

If the learner doesn't answer in a reasonable time, about four or five seconds consider the answer wrong and follow the same procedures you had been doing for wrong answers. Say wrong, tell them the number of volts given the punishment. Go on please with the experiment, please continue.

SPEAKER_06

13:29 - 13:37

Soft, rug, pillow, hair, grass, answer please.

SPEAKER_03

13:37 - 13:39

Go on, teacher.

SPEAKER_06

13:39 - 13:49

360 volts. I think something's happened to that fellow in there. I didn't get no answer. If he was hollering at less voltage, can you check to see if he's all right, please?

SPEAKER_03

13:49 - 13:52

Not once we've started. Please continue, teacher.

SPEAKER_15

13:53 - 14:19

In all, Stanley Milgram ran about 20 different iterations of this study over a span of several years. In this version, many of the volunteers playing the role of teacher showed discomfort, but continued with the experiment. More than half went all the way to 450 roles, even when the screams from the next room went silent, and the student was presumably unconscious. Why didn't the volunteer stop?

SPEAKER_00

14:20 - 14:33

Stanley later debriefs some of the volunteers.

SPEAKER_15

14:33 - 16:07

If you're familiar with the study, you already know that the student in the other room was an actor and not actually given electric shocks. The screams and cries of protest were carefully timed recordings. The only target of the experiment were the volunteers who played the role of teacher, the people who had to administer the shocks. Stanley Milgram studied generated enormous attention and controversy. Admires drew parallels between the experiment and what happened in Nazi Germany. They said, look, people are sheep. They can be easily misled by demagogues and dictators. Critics of the study said, no, those conclusions are vastly exaggerated. They question whether the volunteers actually behave the way the experiment suggested. Some critics said that many volunteers simply refuse to go along. Beyond the academic debates, the study prompted an entire sub-genre of books and movies. Even today, people find the study fascinating and they find it fascinating for one reason. How they ask, good people who know that something is wrong, go along with it. Are such people typical? Is everyone susceptible to such influence? Am I? As we listen to the details of the study, we can't help but ask, what would I do? Would I follow orders and zap the person screaming in the other room?

SPEAKER_06

16:07 - 16:08

105 volts. Out!

SPEAKER_05

16:08 - 16:11

Come on, care about it. I don't want to shock you.

SPEAKER_03

16:11 - 16:12

Teacher, please continue.

SPEAKER_15

16:15 - 16:28

But Vanessa Barnes, a psychologist at Cornell University, realized there was something no one was paying attention to. Everyone was asking what was going on in the minds of the volunteers and how difficult the situation was for them.

SPEAKER_13

16:28 - 16:39

We so often sort of simulate if I was in that Milgram shock experiment, what would I do if I was the study participant, right? Would I actually stand up and go against these directives and say no?

SPEAKER_15

16:40 - 17:00

No one was asking whether it was difficult for the experiment or wearing the lab coat to tell the volunteers to administer electric shocks. To the extent we think of the experiment read all, we might imagine someone who enjoyed putting people in difficult situations, a sort of mad scientist. Venezuela asked a deceptively simple question.

SPEAKER_13

17:00 - 17:06

Was he surprised to see these people going along with this crazy request he was making of them?

SPEAKER_15

17:06 - 17:21

Venezuela's insight was radical. What if you looked at the experiment, not from the point of view of the students screaming in the next room, and not from the point of view of the volunteer administering the shocks, but from the point of view of the person giving the instructions?

SPEAKER_03

17:21 - 17:23

Teacher, please continue.

SPEAKER_04

17:23 - 17:24

I see, going to keep going.

SPEAKER_15

17:27 - 17:31

What if you treated the experimenter as the object of study wants to quit?

SPEAKER_07

17:31 - 17:38

Get me out of here. I told you I had heart trouble. My heart is going to be out of here please. My heart is going to be out of here please. My heart is going to be out of here please.

SPEAKER_03

17:38 - 18:00

The experiment requires you to continue the teacher. Please continue. Please continue. I said before the shocks may be painful. They're not so are you going to so are you going to keep shocking him? So stop, stop, you don't have to keep going.

SPEAKER_15

18:00 - 19:00

Why do so few of us put ourselves in the shoes of the experimenter? Why don't we ask how difficult it was for him to issue those instructions? Why is it when we hear the story, we automatically put ourselves in the shoes of the volunteers, the people receiving the instructions. Vanessa realized that we all naturally gravitate to the point of view of the volunteers and not the point of view of the experimenter because we all instinctively know what it feels like to have other people put us in uncomfortable situations. We think of our bosses, our partners, our co-workers and how the effect our lives and change our moods. We think of the aggressive driver next to us or the other patrons of the restaurant who are so loud and obnoxious that they ruin our meal. refueled buffeted and pushed and pulled by those around us. The one thing we don't ask, what effect do I have on other people?

SPEAKER_13

19:00 - 19:12

There's been a long history of research on social and clearance and persuasion, so we do know a lot about how other people influence us, but we don't know so much about how we experience our influence of other people.

SPEAKER_15

19:13 - 20:19

When we are intensely focused on how the world affects us and not how we affect the world, this can have profound consequences for both good and evil. You're listening to Hidden Brain, I'm Shankar Vedanta. Support for Hidden Brain comes from T-Mobile. The most innovative companies are going further with T-Mobile for business. The BGA of America is helping lower scores and elevate fan experiences with AI coaching tools and 5G connected cameras. Triple A is getting more drivers back on the road fast with location telematics. And the Las Vegas Grand Prix is powering race day operations with 5G connectivity, giving fans and experience at the speed they deserve. This is accelerating innovation with T-Mobile for business. Take your business further at T-Mobile.com slash now.

SPEAKER_18

20:26 - 20:28

You see where your business can go.

SPEAKER_17

20:28 - 20:37

To get there, you may need another 10 trucks. At Sentry Insurance, we put more than 115 years of industry experience to work to help protect you.

SPEAKER_16

20:37 - 20:43

As you launch a new delivery service or expand into a new region and reach your business goals.

SPEAKER_17

20:43 - 20:56

Sentry, right by you, property and casualty coverage is in right or written in safety services are provided by a member of the Sentry Insurance Group's Stevens Point Wisconsin for a complete listing of companies with the Sentry.com. Policy's coverage has benefited his accounts are not available in all states. Policy for complete coverage details.

SPEAKER_15

21:01 - 21:24

This is Hidden Brain, I'm Shankar Vedanta. When Vanessa Barnes was a graduate student at Columbia University, she walks on a study. Every day, she would leave her apartment in the Morningside Heights neighborhood and take the subway from 116th Street to Penn Station. Once she was there, she had to do something she found very difficult.

SPEAKER_13

21:24 - 21:29

Basically, I would just go up to random people on Penn Station and say, hey, we thought this questionnaire

SPEAKER_15

21:30 - 21:38

Vanessa no longer remembers what the questionnaire was about, but she can still recall what it felt like to make such requests of total strangers.

SPEAKER_13

21:38 - 21:59

Yeah, I mean, I still have flashbacks of going down to Penn Station because it was so distressing. I would walk in. There'd be people kind of walking all over the place and then there'd be people just sitting down waiting for their trains. So I'd usually go up to the person who was sitting there waiting for their train, you know, doing whatever they do to kind of occupy their time. And I would say, excuse me, will you please fill out the survey?

SPEAKER_15

22:00 - 22:18

It felt incredibly awkward, stepping into someone's space, disturbing them, asking them to stop doing what they were doing, and to do something she wanted them to do. As Vanessa asks for help and waited for an answer, her palms began to sweat. Her hearts started beating faster.

SPEAKER_13

22:18 - 22:27

It was a really sort of palpable fear that they were going to reject me or worse, right? Say something mean, I don't even know what, but I expected them to say something terrible.

SPEAKER_15

22:28 - 22:36

Looking back on the moment now, it reminds her of another Stanley Milgram study, one that's less famous than the obedience experiment.

SPEAKER_13

22:36 - 22:43

He had his research assistants go on to New York City subways and ask people for their seat.

SPEAKER_15

22:43 - 22:46

Many of his students couldn't complete the task.

SPEAKER_13

22:46 - 22:58

His students started coming back to him saying, I can't do this. This is just so upsetting. This is the most, you know, distressing thing you've ever asked me to do. And he was like, you guys are being babies. I don't understand why this is so upsetting.

SPEAKER_15

22:59 - 23:11

And so, to prove his students wrong, the famous researchers set out for the subway himself. He would do what his students couldn't. Walk up to strangers and ask them for their seats.

SPEAKER_13

23:11 - 23:19

He found the experience so much more distressing than he expected it to be, and all of a sudden he understood why they have been complaining so much.

SPEAKER_15

23:19 - 23:46

Why is it so hard to make such requests? Well, one obvious explanation is that we know that people will reject us, and that rejection is painful. Vanessa remembers being hugely relieved when she was done giving out questionnaires at Penn Station and could head back to her lab at Columbia University. Once there, she and her professor Frank Flynn analyzed the responses to the questionnaire. They noticed something intriguing.

SPEAKER_13

23:46 - 23:50

Frank was like, I can't believe how many people are actually saying yes to you.

SPEAKER_15

23:50 - 24:02

Total strangers disrupted from reading that newspaper or reading a sandwich or watching the crowds of people in the busy station. They were like, sure. I'll respond to your questionnaire.

SPEAKER_13

24:02 - 24:08

We were really surprised by how many people were agreeing in New York Penn Station to do this survey.

SPEAKER_15

24:08 - 25:09

What began as a simple observation turned into something much more important and insight about our minds. Here's the chain of thought that led to the discovery. The reason Frank and Vanessa was surprised that so many people said yes is because they expected people to say no. If lots of people said yes, that meant that Vanessa's fears about rejection were misplaced. Her perception of the influence she actually had on other people was wrong. Like most of us, Vanessa had long felt that others had a big effect on her. As she gazed at the data, she realized that she had a big effect on other people. If she was blind to this power, what consequences could it have on her behavior? As researchers, the first thing that Vanessa and Frank decided to do was test if that personal experience was generalizable.

SPEAKER_13

25:10 - 25:36

We decided to bring participants into the lab and have them do basically what I had done on those number of days. So we brought them into the lab. We said, hey, we're going to have you go out and ask people to as our first step fill out a survey just like I had done. And how many people do you think are going to say yes to you? We made them estimate how many people they thought would agree. Go out and actually ask people. And what we found was that they really underestimated the number of people who would agree to that request.

SPEAKER_15

25:37 - 26:27

So it wasn't just Vanessa and Frank. People in general seem to have a poor assessment of their power over others. People thought that others would find it easy to turn down their requests. Vanessa connected the seeming blind spot in our thinking to Stanley Milgram's famous obedience study. She realized this might be why everyone always saw the experiment from the perspective of the volunteers asked to administer shocks, the people being influenced. No one saw the experiment from the point of view of the experimenter. The person exercising influence. We don't ask, was it hard for him to issue those crazy instructions because we don't identify with people exercising such influence? We think that kind of person must be very different from us because we don't feel we have such power.

SPEAKER_13

26:27 - 26:32

Was he surprised to see these people going along with this crazy request he was making of them?

SPEAKER_15

26:32 - 26:49

So it's interesting when people think about the Stanley Milgram study, I think this is true for myself as well. I always imagine myself being in the role of the volunteer in the experiment, hearing the instructions from the experimenter saying, you must shock this other person. I never put myself in the shoes of the experimenter.

SPEAKER_13

26:50 - 27:08

Exactly. So that was something that we started to wonder about. So we so often sort of simulate if I was in that Milgram shock experiment, what would I do if I was the the study participant, right? Would I actually stand up and go against these directives and say no? But we kind of flipped that idea on its head.

SPEAKER_15

27:09 - 27:36

Vanessa went back to her experience at Penn Station. It felt difficult because she had seen the interaction only from the point of view of her own insecurities. She hadn't seen the encounters through the point of view of the people she was asking for help. From their perspective, an anxious young woman was asking for something trivial. They had to weigh whether to put aside what they were doing and help her for a few minutes. If they said no, it could make them look like jerks.

SPEAKER_13

27:37 - 28:01

It's this really interesting phenomenon where you have these two people interacting with one another and they're both so focused on their own personal anxieties and insecurities and concerns with embarrassment that they don't realize that the other person is feeling that way too. So it's this really interesting situation where being so inwardly focused on your own anxieties makes it so difficult for you to recognize what the situation really is for itself.

SPEAKER_15

28:01 - 28:55

People in these encounters experience what psychologists call an egocentric bias. They are so consumed with their own perceptions that they fail to see what the interaction feels like for the other person. It's absolutely true that many of us are influenced by situations that many of us will do things because the situation prompts it. But there is another problem too and it might be a deeper problem. The people who put us in those situations It's not like they are all powerful gods. They are humans just like us. And they may not realize the extent of the power they have over us. In fact, they may be thinking, I'm sure this portion is going to turn down my request. They might assume falsely that it's easy to refuse instructions. Vanessa realized that this bias could have all sorts of important consequences.

SPEAKER_13

28:56 - 29:10

So what we started looking at about over a decade now ago, we started to look at whether we recognize when we're the ones who are influencing someone else, when we recognize that someone else, for example, can't say no to something that we've asked them.

SPEAKER_15

29:16 - 29:33

Vanessa has now a psychologist at Cornell University. In a series of experiments, she has demonstrated how people are often oblivious to the power that they have over others. In one study, she asked volunteers mostly college students to make a simple request of others.

SPEAKER_13

29:33 - 29:39

We brought people into the lab and we told them you're going to go out into campus and ask people to borrow their phones.

SPEAKER_15

29:39 - 29:46

She walked them through how to approach someone and gave them instructions for what to do once people agreed to let them use their phones.

SPEAKER_13

29:46 - 29:54

They would call us back at the lab and say, I have this person's phone. This is where I'm located. We'd mark it down and then they go on and ask somebody else.

SPEAKER_15

29:54 - 30:03

Before the volunteers went out to begin the study, Vanessa asked them a question. How many people would they have to ask to get three people to say yes?

SPEAKER_13

30:04 - 30:23

And at this time participants are kind of freaked out by this whole thought. They are convinced everyone's going to say, no, they're not going to be able to do the task. And before they actually go out onto campus to do the task, they often would ask us, well, what if no one agrees? Do I come back? What do I do? They have all these concerns about not being able to complete the task.

SPEAKER_15

30:23 - 30:31

What Vanessa found was similar to her own experience at Penn Station. Many more people said yes, then the volunteers expected.

SPEAKER_13

30:31 - 30:40

They thought they had to ask a little over 10. They actually had to ask more like six. In fact, every other person was agreeing to this request.

SPEAKER_15

30:40 - 30:59

Maybe you think the students had a high success rate because they were requesting something trivial. But Vanessa has also conducted a version of the study where volunteers had to ask for something more consequential. Money. For that study, she enlisted the help of the leukemia and lymphoma societies team in training program.

SPEAKER_13

30:59 - 31:17

What people do when they participate in a fundraising activity for team and training is they ask people for donations so that they can participate in some sort of race like a triathlon or a marathon. They get some training, some travel money to be able to do that and the rest of the money actually goes to the organization.

SPEAKER_15

31:17 - 31:28

Vanessa asked participants how many people they would have to solicit to meet their fundraising goals, which would typically thousands of dollars. They estimated they would need to ask about 200 people to meet the goal.

SPEAKER_13

31:28 - 31:36

What we found is that they actually only had to ask about half that. So they only had to ask about 100 people in order to reach their fundraising goals.

SPEAKER_15

31:36 - 31:51

Just as in Venice as phone study, her participants doubled the number of people they thought they had to ask to reach their goal. Their egocentric bias caused them to focus so much on their own anxieties that they ignored the influence they actually had over other people.

SPEAKER_13

31:52 - 32:29

You're thinking about what you're asking. I'm asking this person for money. Will this person give me money? What you're not doing is thinking about what if you were sitting there potentially in your cubicle and a coworker came up to you and said, hey, I'm participating in a race. Would you be willing to sponsor me? If you were sitting there, it'd be really hard to say no to your coworker, right? It'd be really hard to let them down. It'd be really awkward what would you even say? And so people are kind of put on the spot and they find it really difficult to say now, so they go ahead and agree.

SPEAKER_15

32:29 - 32:59

At the University of Chicago, economists John List has also studied the relationship between social pressure and charitable giving. John ran a study where experimenters knocked on the doors of some 8,000 houses in the Chicago area. They were trying to raise money for a children's hospital. John asked me to imagine the scenario from the point of view of the person receiving the request. Let's say it's a Sunday afternoon. I've just made myself something to eat. I'm relaxed.

SPEAKER_12

32:59 - 33:36

You're sitting on the couch watching a football game and you hear somebody knocking on the door. And you think, okay, should I get up or should I stay watching the football games? Of course, a lot of people get up and answer the door. But once they see that there's a solicitor at the door, they say, oh my God, I wish I would have stayed on the couch watching the football game. To late, if they tell the solicitor, no, then they have this very negative or disutility from letting someone down. So they're weighing that off versus just giving them $20 and having them go on their way.

SPEAKER_15

33:37 - 33:51

John added a very interesting twist to the study. Some households were told ahead of time that a fundraising volunteer would come and knock on the door. Others were not told ahead of time. They just received an unexpected knock.

SPEAKER_12

33:51 - 34:15

What we find is that when we warn them, of course, many people just stay on the couch or they leave the house. They never answer the door. The people who do answer the door, they do tend to give money. and much of that is because of altruistic reasons. But the people who we do not warn, they end up answering the door more often and they give more.

SPEAKER_15

34:15 - 34:33

Put another way, people understand how they are going to feel when they put on the spot. They often will go to great lengths to avoid getting in such situations. What this also means is that some significant portion of the money that charities raise might not come from altruism.

SPEAKER_12

34:33 - 34:57

In the case of the children's hospital fundraiser, for example, what you find is that roughly three quarters of the dollars given are due to social pressure and a quarter of the dollars given is actually due to altruism. So very small component of what we observe in our door to door fundraising drive is actually driven by altruism.

SPEAKER_15

34:57 - 35:13

John's research reminds Vanessa of a classic study where researchers set up two boots on a college campus. One booth was clearly asking people for something while the other did not ask for anything. What the researchers found was similar to John's donation study.

SPEAKER_13

35:13 - 35:28

They measured how far away people walked from the booth as they walked by this path. And if people knew that they were going to be asked for something, their distance from the booth was much further than if they didn't think they were going to be asked for something. We just kind of avoid any chance of having to say no to somebody.

SPEAKER_15

35:33 - 35:45

We've seen how egocentric bias can cause us to act in helpful ways to others. We learn phones to people who need them or donate money to charity. Unfortunately though, there's another side to the story.

SPEAKER_11

35:45 - 35:51

They grabbed that headset and they threw it across the room.

SPEAKER_15

35:51 - 36:00

When we come back, the sinister side of our inability to recognize our power over other people. You're listening to Hidden Brain. I'm Shankar Vedanta.

SPEAKER_18

36:17 - 36:22

You see where your business can go. To get there, you may need another 10 trucks.

SPEAKER_17

36:22 - 36:27

At Sentry Insurance, we put more than 115 years of industry experience to work to help protect you.

SPEAKER_16

36:27 - 36:34

As you launch a new delivery service or expand into a new region and reach your business goals.

SPEAKER_17

36:34 - 36:47

Sentry, right by you, property and casualty coverage isn't right to written in safety services are provided by a member of the Sentry Insurance Group's Stevens Point Wisconsin for a complete listing of companies with the Sentry.com. Policy's coverage is benefiting discounts are not available in all states. Policy for complete coverage details.

SPEAKER_15

36:52 - 37:24

This is Hidden Brain, I'm Shankar Vidantan. When we interact with others, we are often intensely focused on how we feel. Our anxieties, our embarrassments, our fears. As a result, we're often blind to the effect we have on others. Their anxieties, their embarrassments, and their fears. Psychologist Vanessa Bonsis studied how such egocentric bias can keep people from asking for help. but that's not the whole story.

SPEAKER_13

37:24 - 38:18

There's kind of the happy story which is that people will help us more than we think and then there's kind of the darker story that people will do a lot of other things for us more so than we think. So we've run some studies where we started out kind of asking people if they could get someone to lie for them. So our original studies involved, you know, just filling something out. We said, what if we just have them ask if they'll, you know, sign their name to say just something saying that you gave them a pitch that you didn't actually give them, just kind of a white lie? And so once again, we had people guess how many people they would have to ask this request of before a certain number said yes. They went out into campus, they asked people, you know, I'm supposed to be doing this pitch. I really don't feel like doing it. Well, you just sign this thing that I gave you the pitch. And again, most people wound up signing it, even though our participants thought that most people would say no.

SPEAKER_15

38:18 - 38:34

As Vanessa says, the volunteers were asking people to tell a trivial lie. And perhaps you could say, what's the big deal in signing a note that says someone gave you a pitch that they didn't? There are no real moral consequences. So Vanessa raised the stakes.

SPEAKER_13

38:34 - 39:25

So what we did is we created these fake library books. We took a bunch of books off my bookshelf and just put some library codes on them. And we gave them to participants and we said, we're going to have you go into the libraries on campus and ask people to vandalize these library books. And so they were to tell people, I'm playing a prank in my friend, but they know my handwriting. Were you pleased just right, pickle, and this library book in Patton? And they left it at that and looked at whether or not people agreed. And what we found is that the people they approached, so they kept track of sort of the things that people said when they made this request of them. And people would say things like, this is wrong. You shouldn't be doing this. We could get in trouble. They were clearly uncomfortable with the prospect of analyzing this purported library book. But they still did it.

SPEAKER_15

39:25 - 39:39

They still did it. And again, that finding went completely against the intuitions of the volunteers doing the asking. People significantly underestimated how much influence they possessed to get others to do something unethical.

SPEAKER_13

39:40 - 39:57

So our participants before they went out and started asking people, they thought about 28% of people would agree to do this, right? So they thought the vast majority of people would say no. But when they actually went out and made this request of people, 64%, a majority of the people they asked actually agreed to vandalize this library book.

SPEAKER_15

39:57 - 40:04

I mean, that's actually pretty astonishing that 64% of people would say yes. I mean, I would not have predicted it would be as high a number as that.

SPEAKER_13

40:05 - 40:28

Yeah, I mean, this was a task we designed and we were like, this is never going to work, right? There's no way people are actually going to agree to do this. And we ourselves were completely surprised that people did agree. As much as it was uncomfortable for them to do this, an ethical thing and analyze a library book, it was way more uncomfortable for them to say no to the person who was asking.

SPEAKER_15

40:28 - 40:59

The scenarios in which egocentric bias could play a role in our behavior seem endless. We share the answers to our homework with a friend who asks to see our work. We don't push back when a colleague suggests bending the rules on a time sheet. We agree to keep a friend's infidelity a secret, even when it makes us uncomfortable. Listening to Vanessa made me realize why there is a vast gulf between what we predict we might do and what we actually do when we are confronted with problematic behavior.

SPEAKER_13

41:00 - 41:30

We look as outsiders at the situation and we say, why would you tolerate this? Why wouldn't you just stand up and say, I'm not going to do this? I'm not going to hurt another person. I need to be able to do my job and you're affecting my ability to do my job. But in fact, the social pressure The concern about offending another person, the social anxiety in that situation is so palpable to that individual that it feels almost impossible for them to stand up and say something about it and reject the sort of behavior that they're encountering.

SPEAKER_15

41:31 - 41:50

Hidden brain listener Anna Aburus, called in with a story that illustrates how egocentric bias can affect workplace behavior. She was training to be an air traffic controller, and so examples of bullying and harassing behavior all around her. She says the trainers had a clear message for trainees.

SPEAKER_11

41:50 - 42:05

Don't be soft. You know, you got to have thick skin to survive in air traffic. That was a common one for sure. You have to have thick skin to survive in air traffic. I've heard that over a hundred times.

SPEAKER_15

42:05 - 42:07

I now recalled one painful incident.

SPEAKER_11

42:08 - 42:42

There was a trainee that was trying to clear an aircraft for landing. And the trainer in that moment grabbed the headset of the trainee. And this headset is plugged in to the radar. And they grabbed that headset and they threw it across the room, which would fly off of the head of the trainee. They would actually tell them, hey hurry up and go grab it so that you can plug back in and clear this aircraft for landing.

SPEAKER_15

42:42 - 42:55

Anna says seeing such incidents made her fearful. She didn't feel she could complain since such behaviors appear to be the norm. Who could she complain to? The people who would themselves acting badly?

SPEAKER_11

42:55 - 43:01

They would just say things like, what the f*** are you doing? If you do that again, that's what f*** God.

SPEAKER_15

43:02 - 43:21

One time when she was directing aircraft, this was in real life not a simulation, she found herself sitting next to one of the trainers, who she says had acted abusively toward trainees. By this point, Anna was no longer a trainee. She was directing to aircraft. One was a thousand feet above the other.

SPEAKER_11

43:21 - 43:40

I got the names of the aircraft mixed up. And I, because I was so nervous, I think. And I descended the wrong aircraft. I descended the one on top instead of the one on the bottom. Because I got those call signs messed up.

SPEAKER_15

43:41 - 43:49

She told the aircraft that was at a higher altitude to descend, directly into the path of the lower altitude aircraft.

SPEAKER_11

43:49 - 44:36

Luckily, the pilot could see the aircraft, so the pilot was just said, no, we're not going to descend. And I immediately knew what I had just done. And I thought today was a clear day. It was clear skies, there was no clouds in the way, there was not any storm clouds in the way, but had there been storm clouds. or had there been some other kind of visual obstruction. This plane would have descended. And they would have hit that aircraft. And I would have been responsible for hundreds of deaths. And it wasn't because I didn't know how to control the traffic I did. And I had done this a million times. It was because of the social stress that I was in at that time that didn't make me think clearly.

SPEAKER_15

44:41 - 45:12

I told Vanessa Barnes what Anna described, how the mere presence of the trainer had disrupted her to the point where she made a mistake that could have been catastrophic. Vanessa said, look, it's certainly the case that there are lots of unethical people who know they are unethical and lots of bullies who know their bullies. Maybe that was the case here. But there is a deeper problem in the workplace that we often forget. The bullies and harassers who don't know that they are bullies and harassers.

SPEAKER_13

45:12 - 45:39

Often when we're the person causing someone else distress, we can't see that distress. It's invisible to us and it's not to let anybody off the hook because clearly it's the people creating this toxic cultures responsibility to kind of fix it and to not cause a things to happen. But there's also this cognitive bias there where we may not realize the extent to which we're interfering with somebody else's performance.

SPEAKER_15

45:40 - 45:46

These same dynamics play out in another common occurrence in the workplace unwanted romantic attention.

SPEAKER_13

45:46 - 46:50

We ran a couple of studies where we asked people about their experiences being asked out at work or asking someone out at work and we asked people to imagine situations where they weren't interested in the other person or the other person wasn't interested in them. And what we found is that people who asked somebody out at work and were rejected thought that it was pretty easy for that person to reject them, right? They didn't think that that person experienced a whole lot of distress and they didn't think that they changed their behavior very much after being asked out. But when people were called situations where they were asked out by someone at work who they weren't interested in, they described feeling obligated to say yes, feeling much more uncomfortable saying no to the person, and they reported doing all sorts of things to try to avoid that person, that the other person didn't realize that they were doing. So, in fact, this little request, you know, we tell people to just go for it and ask this person out, it actually puts a lot more pressure on the other person than we tend to realize when we're the ones doing the asking.

SPEAKER_15

46:50 - 46:56

In some ways, we underestimate the pressure that we exert another people. In some ways, that's the moral of the whole story here, isn't it?

SPEAKER_13

46:56 - 47:12

Absolutely. Yeah, we we underestimate the influence that we have over other people and we underestimate the extent to which asking them for something really puts them in an awkward position because now they have to say no and that's just a really hard thing for people to do.

SPEAKER_15

47:13 - 47:41

Like many psychological biases, the tendency we have to downplay the influence we have on others can have far-reaching consequences. It can keep us from asking for help that would be forthcoming. It can keep us from reaching out and making friends with strangers. And it can also lead us to give into unethical demands or make improper demands of other people. I asked Vanessa how her research had prompted her to do things differently in her own life.

SPEAKER_13

47:42 - 48:25

It has made a huge difference in the little things. So for example, when I was pregnant, if I needed to seat on the subway or on a train, I would kind of stand there and look around and try to look my most pathetic so that someone would give me their seat. Thinking that someone would step up and do it because they were nice, right? But in fact, everyone's all involved in their own stuff. They're not necessarily looking around and paying attention. And maybe they'd be perfectly happy to give up their seat, but they're not going to think of it unless you actually ask. And so I tried to take that into account. So when I was pregnant, I would go up to people and be like, hey, can I sit down? I'd really love to sit. And then of course, people are incredibly happy to just pop up and say yes.

SPEAKER_15

48:26 - 48:40

And what's interesting, of course, is when that happens, you're actually giving people an opportunity to do something nice. It's not just that you're imposing on them, presumably some of them are actually happy to say, you know, I was just writing to work and now I actually got to do this nice thing for this other person. I feel as little warm glow.

SPEAKER_13

48:41 - 49:17

Yeah, absolutely. So a lot of people wonder about the takeaway. So if people agree to help us out of obligation because they feel like they can't say no, then do you really want to ask them for things? But people are really good at justifying their behaviors and ways that make them feel good about themselves. So they may agree to help because they feel like they can't say no. But pretty quickly after that, they're going to be convincing themselves that they help because they're a really wonderful person. And so everyone's going to walk away feeling good about the interaction. You got the help that you needed and the other person gets to feel a good person.

SPEAKER_15

49:17 - 51:27

Psychologists once conducted a light-hearted version of Stanley Melgram's Obedian study. In the 1970s, they had research assistants Stan and the streets of New York City. their jobs to look skyward. At nothing. The question the researchers wanted to know was whether innocent passers by would also stop to look up to see what was going on. They found that when more people were in on the gag, more pedestrian stopped and looked up. I've seen video of that study many times and always found this scene funny. One to 15 people just staring off into the sky. Recently, I rewatched it, and this time, I did what Vanessa had done. I flipped the script. Instead of seeing the experiment from the point of view of the passersby, and asking myself whether I would be similarly influenced, I looked at the experiment from the point of view of the research assistants. Did they expect so many people to join them in looking at nothing? We've seen throughout this episode how all of us as individuals have great power to shape how others behave. If each of us has this hidden power, then collectively as groups, as communities, as tribes, we are going to have even more influence. How we choose to use that influence, that's up to each of us. Hidden Brain is produced by Hidden Brain Media. Our audio production team includes Annie Murphy Paul, Kristen Wong, Laura Correll, Ryan Katz, Audemars, Andrew Chadwick, and Nick Woodbury. Tara Boyle is our executive producer. I'm Hidden Brain's executive editor. We end today with a story from our sister's show, My Uncunk Hero. It's brought to you by team mobile for business. Our story comes from Bethany Renfrey.

SPEAKER_01

51:28 - 53:47

I was 20 years old. My baby girls and I were living in low-income apartments. Most of my neighbors were single mothers like myself. And I remember how overwhelmed I felt that morning. It was a cold day in the apartment. I dragged myself to the sink and it was stacked with dishes with pots and pans that had been soaking because I burnt them all. I didn't know how to cook back then and I would always burn our pans. And I looked at my twin girls. They were 18 months old. They sat in their high chairs. The baby, the newborn was in her swing. I looked back at the sink. And I just couldn't bring myself to do those dishes. And I couldn't look at them any longer. It was a reminder of how overwhelmed I felt in my own life. So I grabbed a white garbage bag and I stacked the dishes in there one by one. I walked out in the rain and I placed it on the edge of the apartment dumpster because the dumpster was full and I came back in and the girls and I left for the day when I got back that evening it was dark and my porch was dark because I didn't even have the energy to change the porch light. But as we were coming in I kind of kicked something. It was a box and so I brought it into the apartment and put it on the table and It was my pots and pans. And they were shining and sparkling. And the girls as blues clues plates and their sippy cuts. And a little handwritten note popped out on a yellow piece of paper. And it said, I've been there before you will make it. I promise you. I don't know which of the single mothers went out there that day and saw that garbage bag. and understood what was happening. But if I saw her today, I would thank her for showing me that we are not alone, and we are not bad mothers, even in our hardest moments. We are surrounded by kindness and understanding, and I'm so grateful to have learned that lesson so early on in Motherhead.

SPEAKER_15

53:56 - 54:37

Bethany Renfrey lives in Sutter Creek, California. When she was 27, she went back to school and earned undergraduate and master's degrees. She is now a legislative director in the California State Senate. Today's my unsung hero story was brought to you by T-Mobile for business. If you enjoy hidden brain, please take a moment and share your favorite episode with two or three people in your life. Word of math recommendations really make a huge difference in introducing new listeners to our work. We truly appreciate your support. I'm Shankar Vedantam. See you soon.

SPEAKER_00

54:48 - 54:48

you

SPEAKER_17

54:56 - 55:25

You have a vision for your business. Your priority might be to expand facilities, or bring in the best talent. At Century Insurance, we listen, learn, and work to understand your business. And your plans to help protect your relocations. As your business evolves, and your vision comes true, Century. Right? Value, property and casualty coverage isn't right to written and take to services are provided by a member of the Century Insurance Group's Stevens Pointless Found Center for a complete listing of companies with a Century.com. Policy's coverage has benefits and discounts are not available in all states. Policy for complete coverage details.